SciELO - Scientific Electronic Library Online

 
vol.20 número5Epidemiología descriptiva de los patrones de hemorragia intracraneal y de las quejas principales motivadoras de TAC cerebral en el Norte de Portugal índice de autoresíndice de materiabúsqueda de artículos
Home Pagelista alfabética de revistas  

Servicios Personalizados

Revista

Articulo

Indicadores

Links relacionados

  • No hay artículos similaresSimilares en SciELO

Compartir


Revista mexicana de neurociencia

versión On-line ISSN 2604-6180versión impresa ISSN 1665-5044

Rev. mex. neurocienc. vol.20 no.5 Ciudad de México sep./oct. 2019  Epub 22-Mar-2022

https://doi.org/10.24875/rmn.19000064 

Review articles

Neuropsychological disorders in juvenile delinquents

Trastornos neuropsicológicos en delincuentes juveniles

Jorge Borrani1  * 

Martha Frías1 

Brayan Alemán2 

Aída García1 

Candelaria Ramírez1 

Pablo Valdez1 

1Laboratory of Psychophysiology, School of Psychology, Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León. Monterrey, Nuevo León. Mexico

2Division of Social Sciences, Universidad de Sonora, Hermosillo. Mexico


Abstract

Juvenile delinquents are young people who break the law. They are usually males of low socio-economic status and low education, and are more exposed to brain damage risk factors, especially drug use, and have a higher prevalence of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder. These characteristics suggest a delay in the development of the prefrontal cortex of the brain, which is related to neuropsychological functions such as language, memory, attention, and executive functions. To assess the evidence of a delay in the development of prefrontal functions, a search was conducted for studies that evaluated neuropsychological functions in inmate juvenile delinquents, comparing them to a control group, and only 14 articles were found with these characteristics. The review showed that, despite methodological issues on task selection and on the composition of control groups, there is evidence that juvenile delinquents have disorders on neuropsychological functions such as language comprehension, visuospatial working memory, selective and sustained attention, and components of executive functions such as cognitive inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and planning. These findings agree with the hypothesis that there is a developmental delay in the prefrontal functions of juvenile delinquents. Understanding the deficits juvenile delinquents have on neuropsychological functions is crucial to design prevention and treatment programs for juvenile delinquency.

Key words Juvenile delinquency; Adolescence; Neuropsychology; Executive functions; Education

Resumen

Los delincuentes juveniles son jóvenes que violan la ley. Por lo general, son hombres de bajo nivel socioeconómico que tienen poca educación, están más expuestos a factores de riesgo de daño cerebral, especialmente el uso de drogas y tienen una mayor prevalencia de TDAH. Estas características sugieren un retraso en el desarrollo de la corteza prefrontal del cerebro, que está relacionada con funciones neuropsicológicas como el lenguaje, la memoria, la atención y las funciones ejecutivas. Para evaluar la evidencia de un retraso en el desarrollo de las funciones prefrontales, se realizó una búsqueda de estudios que evaluaran las funciones neuropsicológicas en delincuentes juveniles internos, comparándolos con un grupo de control y solo se encontraron 14 artículos con estas características. La revisión mostró que, a pesar de los problemas metodológicos en la selección de tareas y en la composición de los grupos de control, existe evidencia de que los delincuentes juveniles tienen trastornos en las funciones neuropsicológicas, como la comprensión del lenguaje, la memoria de trabajo visoespacial, la atención selectiva y sostenida y los componentes de las funciones ejecutivas como la inhibición cognitiva, la flexibilidad cognitiva y planificación. Estos hallazgos concuerdan con la hipótesis de que existe un retraso en el desarrollo de las funciones prefrontales de los delincuentes juveniles. Comprender los déficits que tienen los delincuentes juveniles en las funciones neuropsicológicas es crucial para diseñar programas de prevención y tratamiento para la delincuencia juvenil.

Palabras clave Delincuencia juvenil; Adolescencia; Neuropsicología; Funciones ejecutivas; Educación

Introduction

Juvenile delinquents are young persons that break the laws of a state or nation before being of legal age in that region, usually at 18 years of age1. Only adults can be found guilty of an illegal act since younger persons lack the capacity to fully understand the negative consequences of their actions. Most juvenile delinquents commit only misdemeanors, such as fights, vandalism, and unarmed theft; nevertheless, some of these adolescents get involved in serious crimes such as homicide, sexual assault, organized delinquency, and kidnaping2. Juvenile delinquency, compared to adult delinquency, causes a great part of the global number of lesions, premature deaths, and disabilities3. Juvenile delinquency also reduces productivity, the value of property, and the quality of life of the general population4.

Economic theories of delinquency propose that people respond to the costs and benefits of criminal activity5. Geographical theories focus on the distribution and environment where crimes happen and propose interventions on urban space6. Social theories explain delinquent behavior through the social and cultural conditions surrounding the juvenile delinquent and analyze variables such as peer influence, family relationships, race, ethnicity, poverty, neighborhood, and criminal subculture7.

Gender and socioeconomic status are two social factors that appear in every country where delinquency has been studied. The great majority of juvenile delinquents are males; for example, in Mexico the homicide rate is 10 times higher in males compared to females2. Low socio-economic status is the norm in the juvenile delinquent population8, and it relates to other brain damage risk factors, such as less access to health and education services, greater exposure to illegal drugs, and a greater prevalence of family violence9,10.

On the other hand, biological and psychological theories analyze the characteristics of the person that commits the crime, instead of its relations to the environment. Early biological explanations of delinquency ended up supporting deterministic ideas, particularly Lombroso’s concept of a “natural born criminal.” Deterministic explanations take a single trait that is common among delinquents and portray it as the main cause or predictor of future delinquency. Trying to explain delinquency through a single cause is dangerous because it can bring social exclusion, mass incarceration, or even genocide to a social class, a race or an ethnic group living in disadvantageous conditions. Contemporary biological and psychological theories are mostly not deterministic, but it is common to notice attempts in politics and the media to isolate a risk factor and portray it as the major cause or explanation for delinquency11.

A contemporary non-deterministic biological explanation stems from the premise that, if behavior depends on the brain, delinquent behavior must somehow be related to peculiarities of brain functioning or development. There is neurological and neuropsychological evidence enough to state the hypothesis that a delay in the development of the prefrontal cortex increases the risk of adolescents of getting involved in delinquency12. Some of the evidence in juvenile and adult delinquents that supports this hypothesis is presented below.

Delinquents have a higher level of dopamine than controls; this neurotransmitter appears in the frontal cortex and is related to the display of aggression and cognition13. Adolescents with higher aggression have lower levels of orbitofrontal serotonin, a neurotransmitter related to pain detection and aggression inhibition14. This evidence suggests there are abnormalities in the prefrontal cortex neurotransmitters of adult delinquents.

The incidence of electroencephalographic abnormalities in adult delinquents is between 25 % and 50 %15, which suggests a reduction of the input that the reticular activating system has on the cortex and the rest of the brain; nevertheless, more studies are needed to confirm these findings.

Raine et al.16,17 showed, through positron emission tomography, that a group of adult delinquents had lower metabolic activity in the prefrontal cortex and other subcortical areas, compared to non-delinquents; a literature review on functional neuroimaging in delinquents confirms these findings18. Adolescents living in reformatories with high indices of violence and aggression showed greater activity in the fusiform gyrus, recorded through functional magnetic resonance when presented with violent images17. Nevertheless, another study using magnetic resonance did not find differences in prefrontal activation during an economic task, which suggests juvenile delinquents activate similar neural networks than controls during decision making, despite having worse results19. Alterations in the gray and white matter morphology of the brain have been reported in delinquents with aggressive or violent behavior20.

Most of these studies were done after the crimes were committed; therefore, it is difficult to determine that these findings were not due to the incarceration process. Nevertheless, these results suggest that juvenile delinquents have differences in the structure and functioning of the brain that could result in a delay of prefrontal development.

Some factors associated with a delay in prefrontal development that are common in the juvenile delinquent population are early malnourishment21, drug use22, and traumatic brain injuries23. Lewis et al.23 reported that juvenile delinquents are in a higher risk of having suffered parental negligence, which affects brain development and lowers cognitive capacity. Furthermore, juvenile delinquents have more emergency room visits for severe head or face trauma than other adolescents, which can alter brain development24.

Juvenile delinquents also have problems to learn at a similar pace as other adolescents, especially while learning to read, write, and calculate25 and have a greater prevalence of academic failure11; these school problems are usually interpreted as a delay in the development of cerebral circuits. Juvenile delinquents that have learning problems have been found to have a higher index of recidivism, compared to other delinquents with better academic performance26. Even though not all adolescents with school problems commit felonies27, school abandonment has been related to a greater probability of getting involved in criminal activities28.

In terms of psychiatric disorders, conduct disorder (CD) and oppositional defiant disorder are expected in juvenile delinquents, since some of the criteria for their diagnosis includes arrests and trouble with the police. On the other hand, depression and anxiety have a high prevalence in juvenile delinquents29, these disorders have been related to an alteration of prefrontal activity and its influence in the limbic system30, suggesting a prefrontal alteration.

Juvenile delinquents usually have problems with drug use, for example, 8% of school attending adolescents in the United States report abuse or dependence on alcohol or drugs, but this prevalence increases to 23% in arrested adolescents31. Most juvenile delinquents report having started using drugs the year before their first felony, but others report having committed crimes to sustain their habit32. Drug use can alter neurotransmitter metabolism and the number of synaptic receptors in the brain33 and it has been related to low performance in neuropsychological tests that evaluate reticular, parietal, and frontal lobe functions34, specifically on memory,35 and executive functions tasks36.

Besides having a negative impact on brain development, drug use has been related to a diagnosis of attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (ADHD)37 because these patients have greater indices of use and abuse of substances than other adolescents38. This relationship is not yet clear since it has not been possible to determine if drugs are part of the causes of ADHD or if ADHD patients use drugs as a form of self-medication39.

Adolescents with more childhood ADHD symptoms tend to commit more misdemeanors at a younger age than those without the disorder40. ADHD has been related to a delay in the development of prefrontal areas; therefore, the high prevalence of this disorder among juvenile delinquents41 suggests that these youngsters have a delay in prefrontal development. This delay would manifest as immaturity in neuropsychological functions such as attention, memory, and executive functions42.

These characteristics suggest that juvenile delinquents are an at-risk group for having a delay in the development of the prefrontal cortex. It is possible that this delay manifests as neuropsychological disorders, increasing the chances for school dropout, which combined with poverty, family issues, and other mental health problems leave adolescents with few options, and vulnerable to get involved in criminal groups. Nevertheless, although there are many analyses of these functions in juvenile delinquents, they need to be sorted and discussed to evaluate the neuropsychological evidence of a developmental delay of the prefrontal cortex. Therefore, the objective of this revision is to analyze the evidence of neuropsychological disorders in juvenile delinquents.

Methods

The present study is an integrative review that collects, analyzes, and synthesizes evidence from diverse sources while clearly stating the search criteria. A search for journal articles was performed using combinations of the terms “juvenile delinquency,” “delinquent adolescent,” “juvenile offenders” or “young delinquents” with “neuropsychology,” “attention” and “executive functions” on three search engines: Google Scholar, Medline, and Redalyc.

For an article to be included in the revision it had to be published between January 1980 and April 2018, in English or Spanish, have an experimental group formed by adolescents residing at a center for committing a crime, have a control group of non-delinquent adolescents, and the use of a neuropsychological task. All articles were from peer-reviewed journals, except two theses that covered the other requirements and were included due to their relevance. A longitudinal analysis authored by Moffit and her team was also included because of its great influence in the field; nevertheless, it was not included in the results table. Articles were excluded for: not having a sound neuropsychological assessment and for forming an experimental group through self-reports of delinquent behavior, or with adolescents that were not legally responsible for a crime. With these criteria, 25 articles were taken into account, and in a closer analysis ten were discarded, to finally include 14 articles in this review (Table 1).

Table 1 Neuropsychological studies of juvenile delinquents 

First author Experimental and control groups Paired by Function Tasks and tests Indices Group score comparison (E: Experimental, C: Control)
Abdou (2011) 25 juvenile delinquents 15 adolescent students Age, sex, SES Cognitive flexibility WCST Perseverative responses Males:
E: 33
C: 9.2
Female:
E: 22.6
C: 9.6
Appellof (1985) 23 juvenile delinquents 29 adolescent students Age, ethnicity, sex, SES Cognitive flexibility
Executive functions
Planning
Verbal fluency
Verbal comprehension
Visuospatial memory
WCST
Porteus maze
TVF
Token Test
BVRT
Perseverative responses
Number of categories
Qualitative score
Correct responses
Correct responses
Correct responses
E: 20.39, C: 16.72
E: 4.96, C: 4.76
E: 27.35, C: 22.07
E: 14.87, C: 17.45
E: 36.65, C: 37.62
E: 8.87, C: 9.10
Blanton (2007) 18 juvenile delinquents 14 adolescent students Age, race, SES Language
Executive functions
Language
KBIT subtests
CELF-3
Vocabulary score
Matrices score
Receptive score
Expressive score
E: 86.3, C: 93.2
E: 99.0, C: 101.9
E: 88.6, C: 99.1
E: 90.7, C: 99.6
Borrani (2011) 12 juvenile delinquents 26 normal education adolescents 12 loweducation adolescents Age, sex, education Sustained attention Continuous performance task Percentage of correct responses
DS of correct responses
R of correct responses
Longest sequence
JD: 77.11
NE: 94.96
LE: 80.22
JD: 2.37
NE: 0.90
LE: 2.46
JD: 0.10
NE: 0.20
LE: 0.09
JD: 7.58
NE: 2.77
LE: 9.75
Borrani (2015) 27 juvenile delinquents 27 loweducation adolescents 27 normal education adolescents Age, sex, education Cognitive inhibition Cognitive flexibility Modified
Stroop task
Word reading
Color naming
Switching errors
JD: 23.93
LE: 24.48
NE: 23.56
JD: 53.22
LE: 57.56
NE: 45.74
JD: 68.39
LE: 74.54
NE: 62.19
Carrol (2006) 43 juvenile delinquents 43 adolescent students Age, sex Cognitive inhibition Stroop task Word reading
Color naming
Interference
E: 34.38, C: 43.17
E: 34.93, C: 42.21
E: 51.38, C: 55.29
Caufman (2005) 78 Juvenile delinquents 78 Adolescent students Age, ethnicity, SES Planning
Spatial working memory
Tower of London Spatial Working Memory Problems completed
Strategy score (overall performance)
E: 8.51, C: 8.46
E: 36.20, C: 34.56
Chae (2001) 17 juvenile delinquents 47 adolescent students Age, sex, SES Attention Test of variables of attention Omissions
Commission
RT variability
Decrement in performance
E: 1.56, C: 1.37
E: 4.14, C: 2.55
E: 89.60, C: 89.27
E: 4.29, C: 5.12
Lueger (1990) 21 juvenile delinquents (with a diagnosis of CD) 20 adolescents from a community center Age, sex Planning Cognitive flexibility
Executive attention
Sustained attention
Verbal memory Planning
WCST SMMT AVLT Trail making test Perseverative responses
Number of categories
Number of errors
Learning Time (seconds)
E: 44.50, C: 20.62
E: 3.14, C: 4.58
E: 17.77, C: 9.45
E: 2.69, C: 3.38
E: 39.27, C: 30.71
Olvera* (2005) 16 juvenile delinquents 26 adolescents from the community Age, sex, ethnicity, SES Cognitive inhibition
Cognitive flexibility
Planning
Stroop
WCST
Tower of London
Word reading
Perseverative responses
Number of moves
E: 97.2, C: 107.8
E: 90.2, C: 111.9
E: 104.6, C: 90.2
Poon* (2014) 29 juvenile delinquents with ADHD 29 adolescent students Age, sex, SES Cognitive inhibition
Visuospatial memory Planning
Stroop
BVRT
Tower of London
Interference
Total errors
Number of moves
E: 0.16, C: 0.30
E: 0.70, C: 0.06
E: 0.14, C: 0.01
Romi (2007) 111 juvenile delinquents 31 adolescent students Age, sex, education Language WISC Vocabulary score E: 8.52 C: 10.27
Wolf (1984) 56 juvenile delinquents 48 adolescent students Age, sex, SES Language
Cognitive inhibition
Selective attention Planning
TBNT
Token test
Stroop
Trail making test
Porteus maze
Correct responses
Correct responses Word reading (time)
Color naming (time)
Interference index
Perceptual errors
Qualitative score
Correct responses
E: 28.4 C: 32.7
E: 15.8, C: 17.3
E: 55.0, C: 46.4
E: 75.5, C: 67.1
E: 140.7, C: 129.1
E: 9.9, C: 5.6
E: 18.8, C: 9.0
E: 72.1, C: 72.7

WCST: Wisconsin Card Sorting Test; CELF3: clinical evaluations of language fundamentals; SMMT: sequential matching memory test; AVLT: auditory verbal learning test; BVRT: Benton visual retention test; TBNT: the Boston naming test; SD: standard deviation; SES: socioeconomic status; KBIT: Kaufman brief intelligence test.

*These studies report only their transformed data and the method of transformation is not entirely clear.

Analysis and discussion

The articles were grouped by the neuropsychological functions that are evaluated. The method and results of each article are discussed in the text; the particular tasks and indices that were used and the scores that each group obtained are detailed in table 1.

Language

Juvenile delinquents have difficulties on complex aspects of language, such as verbal fluency43, production of complex sentences, and verbal comprehension, in comparison to adolescents without legal problems and of similar age, race, and socio-economic status to the juvenile delinquent group43-46. These differences were determined using subtests of the Wechsler intelligence scales and the Clinical Evaluation of Language Fundamentals. Comprehension and verbal fluency are related to the functioning of the prefrontal cortex; these results suggest a delay in the development of these areas, and therefore on other prefrontal functions.

Memory

Even though memory is a basic cognitive process, it has been analyzed scarcely. A study reported juvenile delinquents had lower scores on a verbal learning task, which heavily recruits memory, meaning they had a lower capacity to improve their recall of a word list during subsequent exposures, compared to other student adolescents47. A longitudinal study showed a weak correlation (under 0.20) between number of arrests and the score of this same task during adolescence years48.

Working memory is the capacity to recall relevant information for the task at hand and has three components: the visuospatial storage, the phonological storage, and the central executive component. In the spatial working memory task, juvenile delinquents had a low level of performance compared to control adolescents49,50. Other studies that measured visuospatial working memory through the Benton visual retention test report more errors in juveniles, compared to controls paired by socioeconomic status (SES), sex, and ethnicity43,51.

These results indicate that juvenile delinquents have a deficit on the visuospatial component of working memory, which is the capacity to store the position of objects in space; nevertheless, more studies are needed to analyze the remaining components. These results could also be due to an alteration in the central executive component of working memory, in charge of prioritizing the storage of task-relevant information in the visuospatial or phonological storages and, when affected, it greatly disturbs the functioning of the other two components. This component is more directly related to prefrontal functioning, and therefore a delay in prefrontal development could reduce performance on working memory tasks, such as it is observed in juvenile delinquents.

Attention

The majority of the reviewed studies focus on evaluating a single component of attention and employ tasks with indices that are not sensible enough. Olvera et al.44 did not find differences in selective attention when comparing a juvenile delinquent group to a group of adolescents of the same community, age, sex, ethnicity, and similar SES. This study employed a cancellation task with a very low demand that was probably not sensitive to group differences. On the other hand, Chae et al.52 reported that juvenile delinquents had a higher percentage of commission errors, compared to a group of adolescents of normal education and similar SES, which could be taken as index of selective attention; although significant, these differences between groups were small. On this same study, the decrement of performance was analyzed and found to be steeper in juvenile delinquents compared to controls, indicating a deficit on sustained attention in juvenile delinquents.

Another study53 analyzed this process with a neuropsychological task that evaluates sustained attention, which is the capacity to respond at the same level during a prolonged period. Sustained attention is related to prefrontal cortex functioning54. This study found that juvenile delinquents had difficulties on this component of attention, compared with normal-education adolescents; it also found a deficit on sustained attention on an education-paired group, compared to a more educated group. These results indicate that juvenile delinquents have a developmental delay in prefrontal functions and its connections to the reticular system.

Regarding attention, this study was the only one found to consider the education of the participants, and the only one that tried to control this factor by incorporating a control group with the same age and education as the juvenile delinquent group. Through this protocol, differences in attention surfaced between groups of different education, since low-education adolescents (delinquents and non-delinquents) had the same low attention capacity, which suggests that these deficits are related to low education, and therefore not directly related to legal status. Even though attention problems and school dropout appear in juvenile delinquents, they are not a direct cause of delinquency.

Executive functions

The components of executive functions are initiative, planning, prevision, cognitive inhibition, cognitive flexibility, self-monitoring, verification, and correction55. These functions are denominated as “executive” due to their role in regulating other brain functions that have a much more limited and specific operative roles56.

Executive functions are related to the prefrontal cortex, and lesions in the dorsolateral area of this cortex usually produce disorders in cognitive inhibition, the capacity to stop prevalent responses. This manifests as impulsive behavior57, responding to irrelevant stimuli, producing answers out of time, out of context, or that interfere with actions directed to a goal, for example, making inappropriate and offensive commentaries58.

On the other hand, a great part of prefrontal lesion patients shows deficits in cognitive flexibility, the capacity to change behavior based on environmental cues, which impairs the formulation of a different strategy to solve a new problem59. This perseverative behavior makes patients persist on the same response strategy, even though they can acknowledge it is not working57.

Patients with prefrontal lesions also have deficits on planning, meaning they have difficulties setting goals, selecting the action strategies pertinent to achieve them, and executing the behavior sequence that is required to reach those goals56. This deficit affects daily life, making prefrontal patients break more rules, omit necessary behaviors, and perform actions unrelated to the proposed goal, especially when confronting new and unstructured problems56,59. Prefrontal patients commit more mistakes and require more time to solve tasks that evaluate planning, such as the Tower of London and the Porteus Maze60.

Juvenile delinquents have low scores on the performance of neuropsychological tasks related to executive functions43,61. Olvera et al.44 found that a group of inmate juvenile delinquents had worse performance on a Stroop-type task, compared to middle-class adolescents of the same age, sex, and race, which indicates juvenile delinquents have lower cognitive inhibition. Other studies that employed other Stroop-like tasks also reported significative differences in the indices of inhibition51,62.

Another study found that juvenile delinquents have deficits on inhibition using a modified Stroop task64, compared to adolescents of normal education; nevertheless, these difficulties were also observed in a group of low education non-delinquents. Therefore, juvenile delinquents have a deficit on cognitive inhibition, and this deficit is shared between delinquents and non-delinquents of low-education; this implies that this deficit is probably related to their school problems, which is a risk factor for involvement in criminal activities. This study is the only one on executive functions that takes education into account and attempts to control its effect through a group paired by education.

On cognitive flexibility, Lueger and Gill47 found that a group of juvenile delinquents (diagnosed with CD) had more perseverative responses on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test (WCST), compared to a group of adolescents paired by verbal IQ and race, but that had no symptoms of CD and no criminal history. Nevertheless, this result could be influenced by the fact that the control group had a mean age 1 year older than the inmate group. Appellof43 reported that juvenile delinquents had more perseverative responses on the WCST than a group of adolescents of the same age, race, sex, and socioeconomic status. Olvera et al.44 compared two groups, similar to the Appellof pairing, reporting a greater quantity of perseverative responses on behalf of juvenile delinquents. Abdou et al.65 found in a study with male and female juvenile delinquents that both groups had a greater number of perseverative responses on the WCST, compared to the control groups. Nevertheless, even though the groups were paired by age, the control group had more education years than the delinquent group. Finally, Zou et al.50 did not find differences between the perseverative responses of the delinquent group and the control group, and the groups had a difference in education of three school years.

On the other hand, juvenile delinquents with a diagnosis of bipolar disorder or CD needed ten more movements to solve the Tower of London, a task related to planning and prevision44, compared to a control group paired by age, sex, ethnicity, and SES. Using this same task, Appellof43 found a lower quantity of correctly solved problems in juvenile delinquents in comparison with a control group. Zou et al.50 reported differences between juvenile delinquents and controls in a similar task but on another index, the total number of problems solved. Cauffman et al.,49 on the other hand, did not find differences in the quantity of solved problems in the Tower of London compared to a control group, similarly to Poon and Ho51, both using the same index. Lueger and Gill47 took the time to finish the trail making test as index for planning and found longer times in the delinquent group compared to adolescent students.

Since executive functions have a modulating role on the rest of neuropsychological functions, it is possible that the low-performance that juvenile delinquents show on intelligence, language, memory, and attention tasks are explained by their difficulties in executive functions. Nevertheless, the relationship between executive functions disorders and delinquent behavior is not necessarily causal66.

Even though there is evidence that juvenile delinquents have a deficit on cognitive inhibition, cognitive flexibility, and planning, there are contradictory results, which could be due to differences in the conceptualization of executive functions, to poor selection of tasks indices to assess its components and finally, to the lack of adequate control groups67. A common methodological problem in these studies is the lack of control of the participant’s education. This is particularly serious since juvenile delinquents have a lower education level and the great majority of neuropsychological tasks are affected by education, making it unclear if these executive functions deficits are related to the legal status of adolescents or their lower education67. Some authors state that the crimes of juvenile delinquents may be due to their difficulties in executive functions, which make them react inadequately to the environment68. Nevertheless, at least one of the reviewed studies demonstrated that juvenile delinquents have the same problems in executive functions than other adolescents of the same age and education but that had committed no crimes, thus proving that the delinquent behavior of these adolescents cannot be fully explained by their deficits on executive functions. Similarly, not all patients with disorders on executive functions have aggressive or violent behavior67.

It is important to notice that a disorder on executive functions affects almost invariably on academic performance68 and that low academic performance is the main reason for school dropout. Silberberg and Silberberg69 state that low education and school dropout are the factors that most increase the risk for an adolescent to get involved in criminal activities. Nevertheless, it is evident that not all adolescents with school problems or all patients with prefrontal deficits or ADHD become juvenile delinquents, therefore making it dangerous to state a causal relationship between these factors and criminal behavior. Nevertheless, early interventions in these neuropsychological deficits can improve protective factors and deter adolescents from criminal activities.

Conclusions

The literature reviewed shows that juvenile delinquents have neuropsychological disorders on language, working memory, selective, and sustained attention, besides disorders on components of executive functions such as planning, cognitive inhibition, and flexibility. These findings support the hypothesis that juvenile delinquents have a delay in the development of the prefrontal cortex. It is important to analyze how the combination of these neuropsychological disorders and other social factors raise the risk of getting involved in criminal activities, to prevent school dropout and juvenile delinquency.

References

1. United Nations. World Youth Report, 2003:the Global Situation of Young People. New York:United Nations Publications;2003. [ Links ]

2. Reza A, Mercy JA, Krug E. Epidemiology of violent deaths in the world. Inj Prev. 2001;7:104-11. [ Links ]

3. Krug EG, Mercy JA, Dahlberg LL, Zwi AB. The world report on violence and health. Lancet. 2002;360:1083-8. [ Links ]

4. Taylor RB. The impact of crime on communities. Ann Am Acad Pol Soc Sci. 1995;539:28-45. [ Links ]

5. Garoupa N. Economic theory of criminal behavior. In:Bruinsma G, Weisburd D, editors. Encyclopedia of Criminology and Criminal Justice. New York:Springer. [ Links ]

6. Eck J, Weisburd DL. Crime Places in Crime Theory. Crime and Place:Crime Prevention Studies. Hebrew University of Jerusalem Legal Research Paper;2015. [ Links ]

7. Rock P. Sociological theories of crime. In:Maguire M, Morgan R, Reiner R, editors. The Oxford Handbook of Criminology. Oxford, UK:Oxford University Press;2002. 3. [ Links ]

8. Farrington DP. Predictors, causes, and correlates of male youth violence. Crime Justice. 1998;24:421-75. [ Links ]

9. Elgar FJ, Knight J, Worrall GJ, Sherman G. Behavioural and substance use problems in rural and urban delinquent youths. Can J Psychiatry. 2003;48:633-6. [ Links ]

10. Lansford JE, Miller-Johnson S, Berlin LJ, Dodge KA, Bates JE, Pettit GS, et al. Early physical abuse and later violent delinquency:a prospective longitudinal study. Child Maltreat. 2007;12:233-45. [ Links ]

11. Williams KS. Textbook on Criminology. Oxford:Oxford University Press;2012. 679. [ Links ]

12. Ishikawa SS, Raine A. Prefrontal deficits and antisocial behavior:a causal model. In:Lahey BB, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, editors. Causes of Conduct Disorder and Juvenile Delinquency. New York, US:The Guilford Press;2003. 277-304. [ Links ]

13. Raine A. The biological basis of crime. In:Wilson JQ, Petersilia J, editors. Crime:Public Policies for Crime Control. Oakland, California:ICS Press;2002. 32. [ Links ]

14. Golubchik P, Mozes T, Vered Y, Weizman A. Platelet poor plasma serotonin level in delinquent adolescents diagnosed with conduct disorder. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2009;33:1223-5. [ Links ]

15. Mednick SA, Volavka J. Biology and crime. Crime and Justice. 1980;2:85-158. [ Links ]

16. Raine A, Lencz T, Bihrle S, LaCasse L, Colletti P. Reduced prefrontal gray matter volume and reduced autonomic activity in antisocial personality disorder. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2000;57:119-27. [ Links ]

17. Raine A, Moffitt TE, Caspi A, Loeber R, Stouthamer-Loeber M, Lynam D, et al. Neurocognitive impairments in boys on the life-course persistent antisocial path. J Abnorm Psychol. 2005;114:38-49. [ Links ]

18. Bufkin JL, Luttrell VR. Neuroimaging studies of aggressive and violent behavior:current findings and implications for criminology and criminal justice. Trauma Violence Abuse. 2005;6:176-91. [ Links ]

19. Qiao Y, Xie B, Du X. Abnormal response to emotional stimulus in male adolescents with violent behavior in China. Eur Child Adolesc Psychiatry. 2012;21:193-8. [ Links ]

20. Georgieff MK. Nutrition and the developing brain:nutrient priorities and measurement. Am J Clin Nutr. 2007;85:614S-20. [ Links ]

21. Geddes JF, Hackshaw AK, Vowles GH, Nickols CD, Whitwell HL. Neuropathology of inflicted head injury in children. I. Patterns of brain damage. Brain. 2001;124:1290-8. [ Links ]

22. von Geusau NA, Stalenhoef P, Huizinga M, Snel J, Ridderinkhof KR. Impaired executive function in male MDMA (“ecstasy”) users. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2004;175:331-41. [ Links ]

23. Lewis DO, Shanok SS, Balla DA. Perinatal difficulties, head and face trauma, and child abuse in the medical histories of seriously delinquent children. Am J Psychiatry. 1979;136:419-23. [ Links ]

24. Ryan JP, Williams AB, Courtney ME. Adolescent neglect, juvenile delinquency and the risk of recidivism. J Youth Adolesc. 2013;42:454-65. [ Links ]

25. Katsiyannis A, Ryan JB, Zhang D, Spann A. Juvenile delinquency and recidivism:the impact of academic achievement. Read Writ Q. 2008;24:177-96. [ Links ]

26. Blomberg TG, Bales WD, Mann K, Piquero AR, Berk RA. Incarceration, education and transition from delinquency. J Crim Justice. 2011;39:355-65. [ Links ]

27. Mallett CA. Disparate juvenile court outcomes for disabled delinquent youth:a social work call to action. Child Adolesc Soc Work J. 2009;26:197-207. [ Links ]

28. Maniadaki K, Kakouros E. Attention problems and learning disabilities in young offenders in detention in Greece. Psychology. 2011;2:53-9. [ Links ]

29. Teplin LA, Abram KM, McClelland GM, Dulcan MK, Mericle AA. Psychiatric disorders in youth in juvenile detention. Arch Gen Psychiatry. 2002;59:1133-43. [ Links ]

30. Monk CS, Nelson EE, McClure EB, Mogg K, Bradley BP, Leibenluft E, et al. Ventrolateral prefrontal cortex activation and attentional bias in response to angry faces in adolescents with generalized anxiety disorder. Am J Psychiatry. 2006;163:1091-7. [ Links ]

31. National Survey on Drug Use and Health. Substance Use, Abuse, and Dependence among Youths Who Have Been in a Jail or a Detention Center. New York:National Survey on Drug Use and Health;2004, 3. [ Links ]

32. Prichard J, Payne J. Key Findings from the Drug use Careers of Juvenile Offenders Study. Trends Issues in Crime and Criminal Justice. Australia:Australian Institute of Criminology;2005. 304. [ Links ]

33. Rogers RD, Robbins TW. Investigating the neurocognitive deficits associated with chronic drug misuse. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2001;11:250-7. [ Links ]

34. Grant S, Contoreggi C, London ED. Drug abusers show impaired performance in a laboratory test of decision making. Neuropsychologia. 2000;38:1180-7. [ Links ]

35. Reneman L, Booij J, Schmand B, van den Brink W, Gunning B. Memory disturbances in “Ecstasy” users are correlated with an altered brain serotonin neurotransmission. Psychopharmacology (Berl). 2000;148:322-4. [ Links ]

36. Fernández-Serrano MJ, Pérez-García M, Schmidt Río-Valle J, Verdejo-García A. Neuropsychological consequences of alcohol and drug abuse on different components of executive functions. J Psychopharmacol. 2010;24:1317-32. [ Links ]

37. Neighbors B, Kempton T, Forehand R. Co-occurrence of substance abuse with conduct, anxiety, and depression disorders in juvenile delinquents. Addict Behav. 1992;17:379-86. [ Links ]

38. Rösler M, Retz W, Retz-Junginger P, Hengesch G, Schneider M, Supprian T, et al. Prevalence of attention deficit-/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD) and comorbid disorders in young male prison inmates. Eur Arch Psychiatry Clin Neurosci. 2004;254:365-71. [ Links ]

39. Albanese M. The self-medication hypothesis:theory and concept. Psychiatr Times. 2003;20:42-4. [ Links ]

40. Moffitt TE. Juvenile delinquency and attention deficit disorder:boys'developmental trajectories from age 3 to age 15. Child Dev. 1990;61:893-910. [ Links ]

41. Anckarsäter H, Nilsson T, Ståhlberg O, Gustafson M, Saury JM, Råstam M, et al. Prevalences and configurations of mental disorders among institutionalized adolescents. Dev Neurorehabil. 2007;10:57-65. [ Links ]

42. Barkley RA. Behavioral inhibition, sustained attention, and executive functions:constructing a unifying theory of ADHD. Psychol Bull. 1997;121:65-94. [ Links ]

43. Appellof ES. Prefrontal Functions in Juvenile Delinquents (Unpublished Doctoral Dissertation). Syracuse University;1985. Available from:http://www.surface.syr.edu/psy_etd/112. [ Links ]

44. Olvera RL, Semrud-Clikeman M, Pliszka SR, O'Donnell L. Neuropsychological deficits in adolescents with conduct disorder and comorbid bipolar disorder:a pilot study. Bipolar Disord. 2005;7:57-67. [ Links ]

45. Blanton DJ, Dagenais PA. Comparison of language skills of adjudicated and nonadjudicated adolescent males and females. Lang Speech Hear Serv Sch. 2007;38:309-14. [ Links ]

46. Romi S, Marom D. Differences in intelligence between nondelinquent and dropout delinquent adolescents. Adolescence. 2007;42:325-36. [ Links ]

47. Lueger RJ, Gill KJ. Frontal-lobe cognitive dysfunction in conduct disorder adolescents. J Clin Psychol. 1990;46:696-706. [ Links ]

48. Moffitt TE, Lynam DR, Silva PA. Neuropsychological tests predicting persistent male delinquency.Criminology. 1994;32:277-300. [ Links ]

49. Cauffman E, Steinberg L, Piquero AR. Psychological, neuropsychological and physiological correlates of serious antisocial behavior in adolescence:the role of self-control. Criminology. 2005;43:133-76. [ Links ]

50. Zou Z, Meng H, Ma Z, Deng W, Du L, Wang H, et al. Executive functioning deficits and childhood trauma in juvenile violent offenders in china. Psychiatry Res. 2013;207:218-24. [ Links ]

51. Poon K, Ho CS. Contrasting deficits on executive functions in Chinese delinquent adolescents with attention deficit and hyperactivity disorder symptoms and/or reading disability. Res Dev Disabil. 2014;35:3046-56. [ Links ]

52. Chae PK, Jung HO, Noh KS. Attention deficit hyperactivty disorder in Korean juvenile delinquents. Adolescence. 2001;36:707-25. [ Links ]

53. Borrani J. Análisis de los Indicadores de la Atención Sostenida en Delincuentes Juveniles [Masters'Degree Thesis]. Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León;2011. [ Links ]

54. Godefroy O. Frontal syndrome and disorders of executive functions. J Neurol. 2003;250:1-6. [ Links ]

55. Mesulam MM. The human frontal lobes:transcending the default mode through contingent encoding. In:Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. New York, US:Oxford University Press;2002. 8-30. [ Links ]

56. Floden D, Alexander MP, Kubu CS, Katz D, Stuss DT. Impulsivity and risk-taking behavior in focal frontal lobe lesions. Neuropsychologia. 2008;46:213-23. [ Links ]

57. Tranel D. Emotion, decision making, and the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. In:Principles of Frontal Lobe Function. New York, US:Oxford University Press;2002. 338-53. [ Links ]

58. Goldberg E. The Executive Brain:frontal Lobes and the Civilized Mind. New York:Oxford University Press;2002. [ Links ]

59. Zalla T, Plassiart C, Pillon B, Grafman J, Sirigu A. Action planning in a virtual context after prefrontal cortex damage. Neuropsychologia. 2001;39:759-70. [ Links ]

60. Kaller CP, Unterrainer JM, Rahm B, Halsband U. The impact of problem structure on planning:insights from the tower of London task. Brain Res Cogn Brain Res. 2004;20:462-72. [ Links ]

61. Moffitt TE. The neuropsychology of juvenile delinquency:a critical review. Crime Justice. 1990;12:99-169. [ Links ]

62. Carroll A, Hemingway F, Bower J, Ashman A, Houghton S, Durkin K. Impulsivity in juvenile delinquency:differences among early-onset, late-onset, and non-offenders. J Youth Adolesc. 2006;35:517-27. [ Links ]

63. Tung S, Chhabra N. A comparative study on the neuropsychological status of delinquent and non-delinquent boys.Int J Cult Ment Health. 2011;4:121-7. [ Links ]

64. Borrani J, Frías M, Ortiz X, García A, Valdez P. Analysis of cognitive inhibition and flexibility in juvenile delinquents. J Forens Psychiatry Psychol. 2015;26:60-77. [ Links ]

65. Abdou AA, Amer D, Sadek MN. Gender differences in personality characteristics and cognitive abilities in adolescents admitted in correctional institutes in Egypt. Egypt J Psychiatry. 2012;33:9. [ Links ]

66. Golden CJ, Jackson ML, Peterson-Rohne A, Gontkovsky ST. Neuropsychological correlates of violence and aggression:a review of the clinical literature. Aggress. Violent Behav. 1996;1:3-25. [ Links ]

67. Yeudall LT, Fromm-Auch D, Davies P. Neuropsychological impairment of persistent delinquency. J Nerv Ment Dis. 1982;170:257-65. [ Links ]

68. St Clair-Thompson HL, Gathercole SE. Executive functions and achievements in school:shifting, updating, inhibition, and working memory. Q J Exp Psychol (Hove). 2006;59:745-59. [ Links ]

69. Silberberg NE, Silberberg MC. School achievement and delinquency. Rev Educ Res. 1971;41:17-33. [ Links ]

Source of financingThis study had no funding of any kind.

Received: March 22, 2019; Accepted: April 23, 2019

* Correspondence: Jorge Borrani Laboratorio de Psicofisiología Facultad de Psicología Universidad Autónoma de Nuevo León Dr. Carlos Canseco, 110 Col. Mitras Centro C.P. 64460, Monterrey, Nuevo León, México E-mail: jorgeborrani@gmail.com

Conflicts of interest

The authors here declare that there are no conflicts of interest in this article review.

Creative Commons License Instituto Nacional de Cardiología Ignacio Chávez. Published by Permanyer. This is an open ccess article under the CC BY-NC-ND license