Introduction
The varied combination of multimodal sensory stimuli present in environmental enrichment induces biochemical changes that enhance neural plasticity and neuroprotection1. This allows an optimal adaptation of the central nervous system to the changing sensory demand of the environment through enhanced morphofunctional reorganization and synaptic consolidation, which optimizes sensory-motor and cognitive-behavioral learning2. Therefore, an enriched environment allows generating a zone of well-being where the person interacts in an optimal and novel way with a variability of complex stimuli, generating greater experience-dependent neural plasticity versus traditional stimulation3.
The literature defines the environmental enrichment paradigm, from cognitive neuroscience, as a contextual combination of complex inanimate and social stimuli that facilitate sensory, motor, cognitive, and socioemotional skills4. This definition focuses on how contextual enrichment can optimize the adaptation and functioning of neural networks, validating it as a potential low-cost intervention model applicable to educational5, neurological6, and mental health7. The transversality of the intervention model is particularly interesting for patients with intellectual disabilities, who may see their performance affected in these three areas.
Intellectual disability is characterized by deficits in cognitive abilities and limitations in the ability to adapt to the environment and social environment8, originating before the age of 18 years9. Its etiology is varied, encompassing various diagnoses such as unspecified genetic syndromes, Down syndrome, neurodevelopmental disorders, and among others10. Environmental enrichment has been shown to be a viable intervention strategy in animal models of intellectual disability11-13; however, evidence in human models is still incipiently developing14.
In recent years, there have been different review studies that directly or indirectly characterize the effects of environmental enrichment on intellectual disability. There are systematic reviews that highlight the effects of this paradigm on cognitive-behavioral and sensorimotor variables in neurological rehabilitation in adults15 and in animal models of acquired brain injury16. In turn, there have been narrative reviews on the reaches of environmental enrichment on age-dependent cognitive reserve in animal models17-19, on learning in the classroom20 and in animal models in intellectual disability11,13; however, to date, no systematic review focused on the neurocognitive effects of environmental enrichment in intellectual disability in children and adolescents has been identified.
Therefore, the aim of this article was to characterize the neurocognitive effects of protocolized environmental enrichment interventions in children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities. This will provide reliable and updated information that will allow incorporating the principles of this paradigm in the design of public policies and educational, social, and health interventions.
Method
An electronic and systematic search of articles was performed following the systematic review methodology based on the international PRISMA guidelines21, which was developed in two processes. The first aimed to identify the studies to be included and the second to elaborate a matrix with the purpose of extracting information from the studies for subsequent analysis. The article selection process involved five stages (identification, duplicate, screening, eligibility, and bias) which can be seen graphically in figure 1.
The identification stage consisted of searching for articles in the Web of Science (WoS), Scopus, EBSCOhost, and PubMed databases using the keywords “environmental enrichment,” “intellectual disability,” “children,” and “teens,” their synonyms, extensions, and conjugations, together with the use of Boolean OR and AND terms. A search iteration was performed and the filters specific to each database were applied in relation to the date of publication between January 2000 and October 2020, and type of document article (for syntax used by database, Supplementary Table 1). The final search was performed on October 3, 2020. The duplicate stage consisted of eliminating those studies that had a literal copy of the selected text. The screening stage consisted of a review by two independent judges, who were presented with a protocol to evaluate the objective of the review and the keywords to be identified in the title and abstract. Articles that did not contain the key words or did not respond to the objective of this review were eliminated. Articles were also eliminated when two judges considered that they should not be included. Discrepancies were resolved by a third reviewer who decided whether or not to include the article. The eligibility stage involved the downloading and complete reading of the research and the application of the previously established exclusion criteria. Quantitative empirical research that explicitly stated the use of environmental enrichment protocols22 in children and adolescents23 with intellectual disabilities were included in the study. Exclusion criteria considered were as follows: (1) article not available; (2) case study; (3) language other than Spanish, English or Portuguese; (4) qualitative research; (5) adult research; (6) animal model research; (7) theoretical research; (8) does not specify environmental enrichment; and (9) no diagnosis associated with intellectual disability. The bias assessment stage consisted of the review of the entire process by two independent reviewers, considering the exclusion criteria presented. Finally, the process for the analysis of the information from the included studies24-30 involved the aspects described in tables 1 and 2.
ID | Aspect | Description |
---|---|---|
1 | Article | Indicates surname(s) of author(s) and year of publication. |
2 | Country | Indicates the country where the study was carried out. |
3 | Study design | Indicates the methodological design of the study. |
4 | Participants | 4.1 Indicates the medical diagnosis per
control/experimental group. 4.2 Indicates the age range of the participants per control/experimental group. |
5 | n | Indicates the number of participants per control/experimental group. |
6 | Neurocognitive variable | Indicates the neurocognitive variable(s) observed. |
7 | Instrument | Indicates the measurement instrument used to quantify the neurocognitive variable(s) observed. |
8 | Effect | Indicates the results obtained in the study in relation to the neurocognitive variable(s) observed. |
ID | Aspect | Description |
---|---|---|
1 | Article | Indicates surname(s) of author(s) and year of publication. |
2 | Control protocol | Indicates the protocol used with the control group. |
3 | Experimental protocol | Indicates the protocol used with the experimental group. |
4 | Provider | Indicates the person in charge of providing the environmental enrichment. |
5 | Frequency | Indicates the number of sessions per unit of time. |
6 | Duration | 6.1 Indicates the duration of each session. 6.2 Indicates the duration of the entire intervention. |
7 | Follow-up | Indicates the follow-up time after the last session. |
Results
Of the 1064 articles identified in the literature search, 450 were eliminated as duplicates and 465 were excluded at the title and abstract review stage. Of the remaining 149 articles, 115 were excluded due to the following criteria: theoretical study (27), qualitative study (10), animal model study (47), and adult study (31). Thirty-four articles passed to the full-text evaluation stage, where bias evaluation methods were applied with three independent reviewers and the elaboration of a table of excluded articles and reasons for exclusions (Supplementary Table 2). Finally, seven articles were selected for data extraction and analysis of results, through the design of two matrices describing the characterization of neurocognitive effects (Table 3) and the characterization of environmental enrichment protocols (Table 4). The process of identification and eligibility of items described above is shown graphically in figure 1.
Article | Country | Study design | Participants | n | Neurocognitive variable | Instrument | Effect |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Arkhipova et al. (2019)24 | Russia | Experimental | 4.1 CG: Oligophrenia EG:Oligophrenia 4.2 CG: (9-10) yrs EG: (9-10) yrs |
CG: 28 EG: 28 |
Speech Coherence | Glukhov Examination of the Coherent Speech Status in Children with General Speech Underdevelopment (Adapted) | The level of speech coherence had a clinical improvement in the EG, while in the CG, they remained unchanged. Clinical improvements were evidenced in speech correctness, expressiveness and clarity, vocabulary enrichment, improved syntax, and intrinsic motivation. |
Ekins et al. (2019)25 | Germany | Experimental | 4.1 CG: ID EG: ID 4.2 CG: 13.4 ± 1.7 yrs EG: 14.2 ± 3.1 yrs |
CG: 5 EG: 10 |
Cognitive, Social, and Practical Competencies Child Behavior and Emotion Motor Skills | The Heidelberg Competency Inventory (HKI) Behavior
Questionnaire for Developmental Disabilities (VFL-L) The Developmental Behavior Checklist (DBC) German Motor Skill Test (DMT) |
HKI with no significant differences. DBC improves individual behavior patterns of EG compared to cg (p=.007). VFL-L improves behavior significantly (p=.08) in EG compared to CG (p=.345). DMT with significant improvements in EG in 5/8 motor tasks assessed, while CG shows no significant differences. |
Kagitcibasi et al. (2009)26 | Turkey | Experimental | 4.1 CG: RCD EG: RCD 4.2 CG: (3-5) yrs EG: (3-5) yrs |
CG: 165 EG: 90 |
Cognitive Ability | Operationalization of cognitive ability in academic achievement, university attendance, and vocabulary test scores. | Developmental trajectories indicated that children whose pre-intervention CD were mild to moderate, but not severe, benefited from early EE. A significantly higher percentage of the EG attended university (44.1%) compared to those in the CG (26.6%, p = 0.03). There is a trend for participants who experienced EE in childhood (independent of type) to acquire higher educational attainment than participants who did not experience enrichment. |
Kozulin et al. (2010)27 | Italy, Israel, Belgium, Chile and Canada | Experimental | 4.1 CG: NDD EG: NDD 4.2 CG: (4-20) yrs EG: (4-20) yrs |
CG: 49 EG: 95 |
Intellectual Capacity | Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children-Revised (WISC-R) Raven's Progressive Matrices Test | All reevaluated subtest scores were better in the EG, with the difference being statistically significant in the “Similarities” (p = 0.008), “Incomplete Figures” (p = 0.003) and Raven's Matrices (p = 0.019) subtests. Fluid intelligence improves substantially in children with CD exposed to the enrichment program. |
Kramer et al. (2018)28 | United States | Experimental | 4.1 CG: NDD EG: NDD 4.2 CG: 17,5 ± 2,3 yrs EG: 17,5 ± 1,8 yrs |
CG: 35 EG: 47 |
Knowledge Problem-Solving Self-Determination Self-Efficacy Goal Achievement | Project ASDM Knowledge and Problem-Solving Test (Project ASDM Test) American Institutes for Research Self Determination Scale (AIR Self-determination) Disability Related Self-efficacy Goal Attainment Scaling | The EG showed sustained changes in self-determination and goal achievement. There was also evidence of significantly higher knowledge of environmental barriers (p < 0.001), and a greater ability to apply that knowledge to the achievement of participatory goals. There was no evidence of changes in self-efficacy. |
Simó-Pinatella et al. (2019)29 | Spain | Quasi-Experimental | 4.1 CG: NA EG: DI 4.2 CG: NA EG: 15,5 ± 1,8yrs |
CG: NA EG: 28 |
Challenging Behavior | Inventory of Preferred Activities and Reinforcers of
Rueda. Observation/Recording Sheet |
EE had a significant impact (p < 0.05) on the reduction of the three challenging behaviors analyzed: aggressive, destructive and disruptive, with a greater impact on disruptive behaviors. |
Wyman and Claro. (2020)30 | Canada | Quasi-Experimental | 4.1 CG: NA GE-1: CD + ASD GE-2: CD 4.2 CG: NA EG-1: (16-21) yrs EG-2: (16-21) yrs |
CG: NA EG-1: 29 EG-2: 34 |
Social Skills | Test of Adol Social Skills Knowledge (TASSK) Quality of Socialization Questionnaire-Adolescent (QSQ-A) Social Responsiveness ScaleSecond Edition (SRS-2) Adult Version | All participants experienced a significant improvement in social skills (p ≤ 0.01). In addition, students with CD, but not those with ASD, reported a significant increase in friendship engagement (p ≤ 0.05). Overall, the EE benefited youth with CD but students with ASD present greater challenges in applying their new social skills outside of the program. |
ASD: autism spectrum disorder; CD: cognitive deficit; CG: control group; EE: environmental enrichment; EG: experimental group; ID: intellectual disability; NA: not applicable; NDD: neurodevelopmental disorder; RCD: risk of cognitive deficit; yrs: years.
Article | Control protocol | Experimental protocol | Provider | Frequency | Duration | Follow-up |
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Arkhipova et al. (2019)24 | RC | Young Director Children's Animation Studio. EE protocol composed of cartoon creation classes, literary and creative classes, visual activities classes, animation classes and “ABC of acting and sound” classes. | Interdisciplinary team composed of language teachers, computer teachers and graphic designers | NR | 6.1 NR 6.2 NR |
NR |
Ekins et al. (2019)25 | Physical education RC | Physical education RC in combination with Drums Alive® Kids Beats sessions. EE protocol composed of rhythmic percussion, music, movement and cognition through a cross-curricular approach that enhances creativity and critical thinking. | Trained teachers | 2 physical education RC + 2 sessions of EE/wk | 6.1 NR 6.2 7 wk |
NR |
Kagitcibasi et al. (2009)26 | No educational intervention with EE | Educational Intervention Turkish Early Enrichment Project (TEEP). Home and educational center EE protocol for preschool children. Composed of three options: educational nursery, day care center, and home. These three options are accompanied by mother's education. | Trained staff | 60 educative sessions in 2 yrs | 6.1 NR 6.2 2 yrs |
7 y 19 yrs |
Kozulin et al. (2010)27 | Occupational therapy, sensory-motor training and RC | Feuerstein Instrumental Enrichment (IE) Basic Program® (Adapted). EE protocol focused on transforming passive and dependent students into more active and self-motivated learners. Composed of 6/9 items aimed at enhancing mathematical skills, literacy, and social interaction. | Trained teachers | 90 h in 30-45 wk | 6.1 45-90' 6.2 30-45 wk | NR |
Kramer et al. (2018)28 | Mentor, parent and peer-guided planning and execution of a participant-defined community trip | Project ASDM. Multicomponent EE protocol that includes individualized goal setting, group curriculum, and peer mentoring. Each component of the intervention implements problem-solving skills, self-efficacy, self-determination, and participation, with the goal of identifying and resolving physical and social environmental barriers to social engagement | Social worker and trained tutors | 2 sessions/wk | 6.1 120' 6.2 12 wk |
6 wk |
Simó-Pinatella et al. (2019)29 | NA | Enriched Environment ABAB Design. EE protocol composed of 4 phases of 5 sessions each: 1-A baseline without EE. 2-B EE in a playground with a variety of activities. 3-A* again baseline without EE. 4-B* again EE | Tutors and monitors | 5 sessions/wk (Phase 2 y 4) | 6.1 60' 6.2 2 wk (Fase 2 y 4) | NR |
Wyman and Claro. (2020)30 | NA | Program for the Education and Enrichment of Relational Skills® (PEERS). Curriculum-based EE protocol that enhances social and behavioral skills by providing real-world experiences that allow students to project acquired social skills into real life. | PhD student in Educational Psychology and trained teachers. | 2 sessions/wk | 6.1 45' 6.2 16 wk | 2 wk |
EE: environmental enrichment; NA: not applicable; NR: no report; RC: regular classes; wk: weeks; ‘: minutes.
Of the selected studies, developed in European and American continent, five have an experimental design24-28 and two quasi-experimental29,30. We identified four free designed, that is, non-patented environmental enrichment protocols that respond to the characterization of a specific sample: Young Director Children's Animation Studio (YDCA)24, Turkish Early Enrichment Project (TEEP)26, Project TEAM28, and Enriched Environment ABAB Design29. In turn, three patented protocols were identified with standardized procedures, independent of sample characterization: Drums Alive Kids Beats (DAKB)25, Feuerstein Instrumental Enrichment Basic Program (IE-Basic)27, and Program for the Education and Enrichment of Relational Skills (PEERS)30. The baseline diagnoses of intellectual disability addressed in the selected articles are subdivided into oligophrenia24, unspecified intellectual disability25,29, cognitive deficit26,30, and neurodevelopmental disorder with cognitive compromise27,28. The age range of participants ranged from 3 to 20 years. The neurocognitive variables assessed focus on self-determination28, self-efficacy28, intellectual ability26,27, social cognition25,28,30, speech coherence24, motor skills25, and behavioral regulation25,29.
The neurocognitive variables of self-determination, intellectual capacity, and motor skills demonstrated statistically significant improvements on intervention with environmental enrichment protocols compared to control groups without associated environmental enrichment25-27. Specifically, in intellectual ability, significant differences were evidenced in abstract reasoning and fluid intelligence27, and better long-term academic performance26. Social cognition evidenced statistically significant changes following the application of both free designed28 and patented25,30 environmental enrichment protocols. In behavioral regulation, a significant decrease in aggressive, destructive, and disruptive behaviors was observed following exposure to free designed29 and patented25 environmental enrichment. Speech coherence clinically improved in the experimental group exposed to environmental enrichment compared to the control group without associated enrichment, mainly in the subvariables of correctness, expressiveness and clarity of speech, vocabulary enrichment, syntax, and intrinsic motivation24. No clinically significant changes in self-efficacy were present in the selected studies28.
In relation to the design and implementation of environmental enrichment protocols, both the provider profiles, the frequency of intervention and the duration of intervention per session and overall, present high heterogeneity. Providers include trained teachers25,26,30, tutors and trained monitors without specifying profession26,28,29, and interdisciplinary teams24. Intervention frequencies varied between two sessions25,28,30 and five sessions29 weekly with a duration between 45 min27,30 and 120 min28. The total duration of the protocols varied from 2 to 7 weeks in targeted interventions25,29,30, to 1 to 2 years in long-term interventions26,27. Only three studies specified follow-up of participants between 2 and 6 weeks28,30 up to 19 years26.
Discussion
The aim of this systematic review was to characterize the neurocognitive effects of protocolized environmental enrichment interventions in children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities. For such purposes, first, we must differentiate environmental enrichment as a neuroscientific paradigm from enriched therapy or intervention. Environmental enrichment, as proposed by Heidi Janssen, involves creating experiences in which participants can engage in social, cognitive, and sensorimotor activities simultaneously31. This allows for increased opportunities for practice and promotes active participation32. Environmental enrichment merges with activities of daily life and social participation specific to the user's everyday life33, therefore, there are programs contextualized in specific educational, work, or leisure realities. In contrast, the enriched intervention involves the enhancement of a unique dimension, not necessarily fulfilling the simultaneity among the four dimensions proposed by the neuroscientific model. In the enriched intervention, the participant can be an active or passive agent, and the activity is not necessarily designed in a situational context attentive to the user's reality22.
The results of this research demonstrate positive effects of environmental enrichment on different neurocognitive dimensions compared to controls without enrichment in children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities, which is justified based on findings in the field of cognitive neuroscience and neurobiology. These disciplines have demonstrated that the environmental enrichment paradigm induces varied neural plasticity responses in the central nervous system in both animal and human models, ranging from functional cognitive enhancement to potentiation of synaptic plasticity, adult hippocampal neurogenesis, synaptogenesis, and modulation of gene expression34,35. The diversity of multimodal sensory inputs implicit in environmental enrichment induce epigenetic changes that enhance neural plasticity and neuroprotection through upregulation of glutamatergic and GABAergic tone, and upregulation of angioglioneurins such as BDNF, NGF, IGF-1, VEGF, and EPO36.
The horizontal improvement of different neurocognitive variables through environmental enrichment protocols not only sustains an improvement in the cognitive functioning of children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities but, in turn, also provides them with tools based on the contextual practice of activities of daily living and social participation that facilitate their effective educational, labor, and social inclusion. In fact, social models of inclusion theoretically respond to the paradigm of environmental enrichment, as it implies the interaction of sensory, motor, cognitive, and socioemotional factors37. The intrinsic contextualization of the paradigm to the situational reality of the participants encourages the development of neurocognitive dimensions applied to the context itself, which facilitates its extrapolation to the user's daily life.
The only neurocognitive variable that did not show positive changes was self-efficacy. Self-efficacy is defined as a person's belief in his or her ability to achieve certain actions, which will influence thoughts about him or herself38. It is interesting to hypothesize why self-efficacy did not improve in adolescents with intellectual disabilities who participated in the Project Team protocol28. The study suggests that the non-variation in self-efficacy responds to the characteristics of the assessment instrument used; however, let us recall that children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities make external attributions of their successes (“I did well just by luck”) and internal attributions of their failures (“I'm just slow”) mainly due to a history of failed experiences with the environment. This makes them internalize a negative self-image and develop lower self-efficacy39, which in adolescence may be less susceptible to change.
In this study, it can be observed that the design and execution of the environmental enrichment protocols are irregular at the time of establishing providers, frequency and duration. The provider of environmental enrichment is not technically defined, since, strictly speaking, the multimodality of the model and the range of contextual applications imply the formation of inter- and transdisciplinary teams for its optimal design and execution. The disparity in the frequency and duration of the sessions unfortunately responds to the lack of consensus in the literature on the methodological design and dosage of environmental enrichment paradigms40, which is still a matter of research.
This systematic review presents a number of methodological limitations, such as including only papers published in English, Spanish, and Portuguese. The varied etiology of intellectual disability also represents a limitation per se, as it increases the plurality of the sample of studies analyzed. In addition, the methodological design, the environmental enrichment protocols, and the neurocognitive variables evaluated are heterogeneous, respond to different situational contexts, and include the baseline bias of the instruments used to evaluate.
Conclusions
The application of environmental enrichment protocols in children and adolescents with intellectual disabilities has a positive impact on neurocognitive variables associated with intelligence, communication, socialization, and motor skills. The environmental enrichment paradigm is supported by scientific evidence in animal and human models. In turn, its guidelines encourage effective inclusion in activities of daily living and social participation of participants; however, it has methodological limitations that should be addressed in future research.
This systematic review is projected as a prelude to future research to better elucidate the benefits of environmental enrichment in intellectual disabilities. This will allow the foundation of base guidelines for public policies and programs of social, educational, labor, and health intervention that favor this profile of patients.