Introduction
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) is a range of modified behavioral displays that become conspicuous in children about 2-3 years old. The most remarkable behaviors include decreased social interaction, language impairments, and repetitive or stereotyped movements. However, there is also the prevailing attribute allied to the physiology of sensory processing. Since the seminal work of Kanner1, it was stated that in autism loud noises trigger panic or emotional crisis, and to date, it is known that these responses are related to a multisensory dysfunction of the auditory, visual, oral, and cutaneous pathways2. Touch is a powerful stimulation to the skin with the potency to rewire neural circuits that are reflected as modifications in social behavior during development and adulthood3. In animal studies, we have shown that toxic effect to cutaneous C and Ad afferent fibers during development results in increased motor patterns as grooming or scratching4 indicating repetitive movements and suggesting that these cutaneous afferent fibers could be compromised in the stereotyped movements, as those seen in autism. On the other hand, some authors reported that foot massage triggers neuroendocrine responses that could be therapeutic for autism5. Thus, the literature is now becoming plenty of data on sensory processing, suggesting that cutaneous sensitivity may be another central core symptom in autism6. Hence, we argue that it is important for the quality of life in autism to develop some strategies to modify the skin sensitivity. Considering this, we investigated whether the tactile threshold of autistic boys could be modified as a consequence of long-term therapy of skin and sensory stimulation.
Methods
This was a longitudinal study in which all participants were tested continuously for 1 year. The protocol was approved by the Ethics Committee of the Brain Research Center at our university, and procedures were based on the Helsinki Declaration. Parents of the participants received a thorough explanation of the procedures and, on acceptance, signed an informed consent to allow their boys to be included in the study.
Participants
Six boys with autism from 5 to 12 years of age were included in the study; all of them in the Severity Level 1 according to DSM-56. Kids received stimulation with bubble paper and a massager, twice a week for 1 year. In each test, the bubble paper was used for feet sensory stimulation. A carpet of 2 × 2 m of bubble paper was placed on the floor and kids were asked to walk on in socks for 2 min trying to burst the bubbles. Bursting of bubbles produced both feet tactile stimulation and auditory stimulation along with apparent emotional responses. Immediately after the bubble paper activity, boys were asked to sit on a chair and a handheld massager (Homedics, Mexico) was used to stimulate the forearm and the face cheek with a level 2 of intensity for 2 min each area and in random left or right side. Following the massage, boys were asked to close their eyes and the forearm and cheek were stimulated with von Frey fibers of different size to determine the cutaneous sensitivity threshold. Boys were asked to indicate perception of the fiber by pointing the stimulated area with a finger. A cohort group of 10 boys with typical development was included in the study as controls and was analyzed under the same procedures.
The number of sessions required to (1) remove the shoes, (2) accept the massage, or (3) close the eyes on request, was analyzed with the Long-rank Mantel-Cox test, and is represented in a survival curve (Fig. 1). Cutaneous sensitivity (threshold) was assessed every month by detecting the smallest von Frey fiber that evoked a skin sensation. The size of von Frey fibers and the session were analyzed with a Pearson r correlation and represented in a graph with the median and interquartile range (Fig. 2). The fact that typical developed boys responded right from the first test and perceived the smallest fiber, precluded their inclusion in the statistics. Analysis was developed with Prism 8 (GraphPad Software, USA), and significant differences were indicated when p<0.05.
Results
The first challenge was related to the lack of propensity of the boys to remove their shoes on request and walk on the bubble paper, to accept the massager, or to close their eyes for skin detection of the von Fray fibers. However, boys responded positively after a continuous period of being asked for the procedures. Fig. 1 shows that 10 tests were required for all boys to get the disposition for the three challenges, all of them in a similar acceptance curve, with no significant differences among them.
The different sensitivity tests in each month were compiled into a single value for analysis to get 12 values for the year. The smallest von Frey fiber in our set has a value of 1.6 and the largest of 6.65, with a total of 20 fibers of different size. Data showed that boys with typical development perceived the smallest fiber in the first test, whereas autistic boys perceived fibers with the average size of 4.0. Longitudinal curves showed that autistic boys modified their threshold perception during consecutive tests. At the end of the year, their perception was similar to those of typically developed boys in both the forearm and cheek (Fig. 2).
Discussion
The incorporation of autistic boys into new tasks is a challenge due to their strong predisposition to routines. However, we have previously shown that motor and cognitive improvements are possible following long-term stimulation with virtual sports7. Now, to accomplish the purpose of the present study, it was a requirement that all kids were able to remove their shoes, walk and press bubble paper, sit and keep their eyes closed, and point the place where they detected the von Frey fiber. Although they eventually did it successfully, it was not immediate and took them about 5 weeks to agree follow instructions. Thus, responding to procedural instructions is not an unattainable challenge for autistic boys, although they take time to master the task. By contrast, boys with typical development followed instructions right from the 1st day. Thus, one speculation is that the kind of repeated stimulation provided to autistic kids in the present study improves the function of brain regions involved in attention, decision-making, habits, and procedural memory, such as the prefrontal cortex, basal ganglia, and cerebellum, among others8-10.
On the other hand, it is known that skin sensitivity is a remarkable function that allows a wide perception of the environment and stimulates the development of appropriate neural circuits in the central nervous system. Our data showed that at the beginning, the autistic boys had a higher threshold to perceive von Frey fibers. Interestingly, due to the constant engagement in both sensory and the bubble-induced emotional stimulation, their threshold was reduced to typical levels in both the forearm and cheek. Thus, once again, a speculation is that stimulation as provided here successfully modified the function of some parameters of the somatosensory neural circuits. Starting at the skin level, it is known that typical and autistic kids have a similar histology and are similarly prone to pediatric injuries11. In addition, the skin conductance appears to be equivalent in autistic and typical children, which allows the use of this parameter to measure social anxiety and the impact of animals to induce anxiolytic effects in autistic kids12. Furthermore, skin massage seems to be relevant to manage sleep problems in autism13, suggesting that skin-induced changes might occur at central levels.
Indeed, the somatosensory cortex functional connectivity is abnormal in autism14. Thus, we hypothesize that at the beginning of this study, the autistic kids responded to altered skin-to-cortex functional connection, which was probably rewired following a prolonged period of stimulation. Hence, our guess is that those circuits became activated in their potency for neural plasticity, considering that this feature is triggered by sensory experiences15. Further research is needed to explain how such plasticity response could be correlated to the observed behavior. We argue that skin stimulation is a channel for information that is relevant for social behavior16. In fact, social neuroscience, that is, the study of the neural basis of social behavior, has already proposed several brain structures underlying social interactions17. Some of them, as the insular cortex, is modified significantly by tactile afferents from the skin in response to pleasant and social relevant stimuli18,19. Thus, we suggest that the kind of continuous stimulation used in this study was strong enough to modify the circuits and improve the tactile sensation in these boys with autism. Those changes might be relevant to improve perception of the general environment and consequently improve social behavior, but this statement still deserves further studies.