Introduction
In patients with schizophrenia, language disorder has been considered a diagnostic indicator of the illness1. Particularly, abnormalities in connections between words and concepts are the most representative sign of thought disorder2-4.
Some studies have showed that schizophrenic patients have deficits in tasks involving semantic relations between words5,6. Building sentences with highly semantically related words lead to fast activation of their inner representation in working memory (WM)7. Sentence production involves cognitive processes that are based on the integration of activity in multiple brain regions, particularly associative cortical sites8,9.
One technique widely used to study brain mechanisms of cognitive impairments in patients with mental disorders is electroencephalography (EEG)10. EEG studies indicate that between 50% and 60% of patients with schizophrenia show slowing of alpha rhythm, although 15% of them show beta activity between 13 Hz and 18 Hz and 10% of the patients have paroxysmal EEG changes11.
Some authors have used EEG to describe the brain functioning that underlies cognitive task performance in schizophrenic patients12. Most of the symptoms are evident while patients are solving daily life problems; cognitive researchers are aware of this problem and have developed experimental conditions that simulate life demands, such as arithmetical problem-solving13; memory retrieval14; syntactical language organization, and communication fluency15.
In this study, we analyzed the EEG during sentence production in schizophrenic and healthy men. Our objective is to identify possible changes in brain electrical activity in healthy and schizophrenic subjects during the construction of sentences using word pairs with high and low semantic relatedness. We predicted that sentence production using low relatedness word pairs requires more verbal processing and WM capacity than using high relatedness word pairs, and this would be associated to differences in EEG patterns in schizophrenic patients compared to healthy men.
Methods
The study was carried out in accordance with the Helsinki Declaration and was approved by the Faculty of Psychology Institutional Review Board at the Universidad Autónoma de Puebla (Puebla, Mexico).
Participants
We analyzed the brain electrical activity of 11 men in the acute phase of disorganized schizophrenia and 15 healthy men. Patients were recruited from the Rafael Serrano Psychiatric Hospital in Puebla, México, and healthy men were selected by advertisement. All subjects were right-handed according to the Annette scale16. Mean age of healthy men was 32.5 years (standard deviation [SD] = 4.2) and 31.7 years (SD = 3.2) for schizophrenic subjects. Educational level of healthy men was 6.5 years (SD = 2.3) and schizophrenic subjects was 6.1 (SD = 1.4). Patients’ mean age of schizophrenia onset was 31.3 (SD = 0.6) and the mean duration of illness was 6.0 months (SD = 0.1).
All of the patients had between mild and moderate illnesses according to their scores on the Scale for the Assessment of Positive Symptoms17 (mean score = 16.5, SD = 12.8, range = 0.41). Patients were evaluated 1 day after they were admitted to the hospital to avoid changes in their clinical status. All participants were native, monolingual Spanish speakers who never learned any other language. No history of head trauma, substance abuse, or addiction was recorded. After a complete description of the study, written informed consent was obtained from all participants and their relatives.
Stimuli and task
Thirty pairs of highly related words (e.g., table chair) and 30 pairs of low-related words (e.g., dog lock) were collected from the previous work18. Each participant was instructed to construct a sentence using the pair of words involved without changing the order of words or modifying the word itself (e.g., chairs instead of chair). We presented 33 pairs of words for each of the two conditions: low-related and high-related words.
Experimental procedure
Each subject received the instruction that after hearing the pair of words, they must construct a sentence using the pair of words without changing them. The experimental session began by asking the participant to close his eyes. Over the next 5 min, the participants sat with their eyes closed to record resting brain activity. Each test began with the presentation of one pair of words, previously recorded, through headphones. When the subject was ready to state his sentence, he began to speak. The next pair of words was given 5-6 s after the end of sentence articulation. Pairs of high- and low-related words were presented in pseudorandom order.
EEG data acquisition and analysis
EEG was recorded with an EEG device (Nicolet Viking EEG) according to the international 10-20 system from the following leads: Fp1, Fp2, F3, F4, F7, F8, Fz, T3, T4, T5, T6, C3, C4, Cz, P3, P4, Pz O1, O2, and Oz referred to linked earlobes. The sampling rate was 128 Hz. Amplifiers had a bandpass from 0.5 Hz to 30 Hz. For analysis, cerebral electrical activity was selected during the period from the beginning of word presentation to the start of the first oral articulation in each word pair. This period was defined in EEG with physiological markers (quick mouth movement). The sentences were recorded for posterior linguistic analysis.
We eliminated those EEG epochs with movement artifacts. Electromyography and EOG derivative from the movement of the chin, mouth, and eyes was indicated and marked. The final analyzed EEG period was 120 s in all participants. We used the fast Fourier transformation to estimate the power spectra. We analyzed the absolute power EEG rhythms in the following frequencies bands: delta (1-3.5 Hz), theta (4-7.5 Hz), alpha (8-12.5 Hz), beta 1 (13-18.5 Hz), beta 2 (19-24.5), and beta 3 (25-30 Hz).
Behavioral data analysis
The analysis is based on the comparison of values between the group of healthy subjects and the group of patients with schizophrenia. The response variables were the latencies (time in seconds) and length of sentences (number of words) constructed using high and low relation word pairs. Due to the different number of subjects in each group, we used the Mann–Whitney U test to compare the latency between groups and between the two stimulus conditions (high and low word pair relatedness). For statistical analysis and graphical representations, we used the program Prism 5 for Windows (GraphPad Software Inc., San Diego, USA, 2002).
Results
In healthy subjects, the latency for constructing sentences with high relatedness word pairs was 5.1 s (± 0.37 SD) and with low relatedness pairs was 7.4 s (± 0.52 SD). Subjects with schizophrenia constructed sentences using pair of high relatedness word pairs in 6.7 s (± 0.30 SD) and low relatedness pairs in 10.1 s (± 0.97 SD).
Kruskal–Wallis (KW) test showed significant differences in the latency to produce sentences (KW = 47.75, p < 0.0001), post hoc Dunn’s multiple comparison tests showed that this difference depended only on word pair relatedness (Fig. 1).
We counted the number of words utilized to construct sentences. Healthy subjects incorporated 5.86 (± 0.91 SD) words in sentences with high relatedness word pairs and used 7.8 words (± 0.91 S.D) to form sentences with low relatedness pairs. Subjects with schizophrenia incorporated 7 (± 1.0 S.D) words in sentences with high relatedness pairs and 10.18 words (± 1.3 SD) in sentences with low relatedness pairs.
KW test showed significant differences in the number of words to construct sentences (KW = 35.92, p < 0.0001), post hoc Dunn’s multiple comparison tests showed that this difference depended only on word pair relatedness (Fig. 2).
EEG data
EEG data were compared between groups using the Mann–Whitney U-test to assess the statistical significance of the difference in the absolute power of each EEG band. During the construction of sentences with high relatedness word pairs, healthy subjects showed significant higher absolute power in delta and alpha rhythm than schizophrenic patients. However, schizophrenic patients showed significantly higher absolute power in theta, beta 1, beta 2, and beta 3 rhythms compare to healthy subjects (Fig. 3).
During the construction of sentences with low relatedness word pairs, schizophrenic patients showed significant higher absolute power in delta, theta, and beta rhythms, while healthy subjects exhibited significantly higher alpha rhythm (Fig. 4).
Topographical analysis
Construction of sentences with highly related word pairs induced a different distribution of absolute power between the control and schizophrenic groups. The absolute power of alpha rhythm was increase in healthy subjects over the occipital leads. The absolute power of theta and beta rhythms is increase over the frontal and central leads in schizophrenic patients (Fig. 5).
Construction of sentences with low relatedness word pairs also induced different distribution of absolute power between the groups. In healthy subjects, we found enhancement of alpha rhythm, especially in occipital regions, as well as theta power in frontal areas. Schizophrenic patients showed an increase in absolute power in theta band in frontotemporal areas. In addition, a diffuse increase of absolute power in beta band appeared in schizophrenic patients (Fig. 6).
Discussion
There is extensive literature about deficits in verbal functioning in schizophrenia, showing various abnormalities in verbal production, comprehension, and cerebral lateralization of language19. Several functional magnetic resonance imaging studies using language paradigms also show abnormal processing in verbal fluency tasks and loss of left hemisphere lateralization in the temporal and frontal lobes of patients with chronic schizophrenia20.
Some authors Ford and Mathalon21 have argued that EEG measures of coherence indicate interdependence of activity in the frontal speech production and temporal speech reception areas during speech in control subjects, but not in schizophrenia patients. These data suggest that a corollary discharge from frontal areas fails to alert the auditory cortex that they are self-generated, leading to the misattribution of inner speech to external sources.
Wynn et al.22 found no differences between schizophrenic patients and controls in the ability to form global objects from local elements, during visual integration tests. This is relevant to our tests because we asked patients and control subjects to form coherent sentences (global object) from isolated words (local elements). The cognitive effort required for that kind of tasks involves information sorting (word pairs), to generate a coherent order. Schizophrenic patients have difficulty maintaining thematic consistency. In our study, patients formed sentences with no morphological deficits, but which were thematically incoherent. To construct a sentence, using pairs of word, it is necessary to maintain the words in the short-term memory while seeking a context in which to put them, as well as constant monitoring to avoid mistakes23. Short-term memory and monitoring are functions associated with the work of the frontotemporal and frontoparietal cortex24-26.
Expression of language is a complex process and involves a distributed network in cortical regions. Our results demonstrate that when control subjects form sentences with highly related word pairs, absolute power over theta and beta bands is more expressive on frontopolar and frontal-inferior regions, respectively. Lin et al.27 suggested that theta power increases during longer latencies; furthermore, alpha-band oscillations are suggested to be closely associated with attentional selection, specifically anticipatory and temporal attention28.
In schizophrenic patients, the absolute power of the theta band increased in the frontotemporal regions in both hemispheres with an extension to the right parietal region. The beta band also increased diffusely. Doesburg et al. suggested that oscillatory synchronization and cross-frequency interactions are mechanisms of functional integration among distributed brain areas that support expressive language processing29.
Theta activity has been found over frontal regions when subjects form images during mental task performance30. We believe that a sentence forming task involves organizing images, but in more complex tasks, the images may be less structured. On the other hand, beta activity is associated with sensory information analysis and decision-making because the task implies a goal-oriented behavior. The assumption behind this idea is that the motor system initiates active movement preparation on the reception of stimulus input31. Language production in schizophrenia is disordered and filled with irrelevant pieces of information and derailments. Such erratic discourse may be linked to the inability to use pragmatic rules, attention, action planning, ordering, and sequencing32.
Finally, we consider that sentence production using high and low relatedness word pairs could be used to explore different cognitive process, such attention, WM, executive functions, as well as the search for coherence. We believe that in the area of neuropsychological rehabilitation, particularly for schizophrenic patients, it is necessary to create methods to solve problems in discourse construction, looking into different cognitive processes that are required for generating meaning in patients’ speech.
Conclusion
There are significant differences in behavior and EEG activity during sentences production, using low and high relatedness words. Specifically, absolute power is increased in patients with schizophrenia over the theta, and beta rythms. We consider that producing sentences with high or low relatedness words could be used for new experimental paradigms.
Differences in EEG patterns in schizophrenic patients compared to healthy men.